The Cognitive Benefits of Speaking Multiple Languages

Authors

  • Rahma Daud Pwani University

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.47941/ejl.2055

Keywords:

Cognitive Reserve Theory, Executive Control Theory, Bilingual Education, Cognitive Flexibility, Multilingualism

Abstract

Purpose: The general objective of this study was to analyze the cognitive benefits of speaking in multiple languages.

Methodology: The study adopted a desktop research methodology. Desk research refers to secondary data or that which can be collected without fieldwork. Desk research is basically involved in collecting data from existing resources hence it is often considered a low cost technique as compared to field research, as the main cost is involved in executive’s time, telephone charges and directories. Thus, the study relied on already published studies, reports and statistics. This secondary data was easily accessed through the online journals and library.

Findings: The findings reveal that there exists a contextual and methodological gap relating to the cognitive benefits of speaking in multiple languages. Preliminary empirical review revealed that bilingualism and multilingualism significantly enhanced various cognitive functions, including cognitive flexibility, executive functioning, problem-solving skills, creativity, and social cognition. Bilingual individuals demonstrated superior mental agility, better working memory, and attentional control, which were beneficial from early childhood through older age, potentially slowing cognitive decline and reducing the risk of neurodegenerative diseases. Additionally, bilingualism fostered innovative thinking and problem-solving abilities, as well as better understanding and empathy in social interactions, highlighting the importance of promoting bilingualism for cognitive development, academic achievement, and social integration.

Unique Contribution to Theory, Practice and Policy: The Cognitive Reserve Theory, Executive Control Theory and Vygotsky’s Sociocultural Theory may be used to anchor future studies on the cognitive benefits of speaking in multiple languages. The study recommended enhancing Cognitive Reserve Theory and refining Executive Control Theory by integrating bilingualism as a critical factor. Practically, it highlighted the importance of incorporating bilingual education programs in schools and promoting lifelong bilingualism through adult education and community programs. Policy recommendations included supporting bilingual education policies, promoting multilingualism in public services, and encouraging research and development in the field of bilingualism and cognitive science. These recommendations aimed to leverage the cognitive benefits of bilingualism to improve educational outcomes, cognitive health, and community inclusiveness.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

References

Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2014). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80(2), 207-245. doi:10.3102/0034654314555567

Bak, T. H. (2016). Cooking pasta in La Paz: Bilingualism, bias and the replication crisis. Cortex, 86, 186-188. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2016.06.013

Bak, T. H., & Alladi, S. (2014). Bilingualism, dementia and the tale of many variables: Why we need to move beyond the Western world. Cortex, 54, 222-224. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2014.02.014

Bialystok, E., & Barac, R. (2012). Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122(1), 67-73. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003

Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (2012). Cognitive and linguistic processing in the bilingual mind. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(3), 148-152. doi:10.1177/0963721412448851

Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning in early childhood. Cognition, 130(3), 278-288. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015

Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2008). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282-298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x

Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2014). Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282-298. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x

Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2012). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113(2), 135-149. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001

Fan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D. (2015). The exposure advantage: Early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychological Science, 26(7), 1090-1097. doi:10.1177/0956797615574699

Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., & Viñas Guasch, N. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschool children through older adults on card sorting, Simon, and metalinguistic tasks. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 11. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011

Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515-530. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.796377

Kharkhurin, A. V. (2012). Multilingualism and creativity. Multilingual Matters. doi:10.21832/9781783090009

Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bogulski, C. A., & Valdes Kroff, J. R. (2015). Juggling two languages in one mind: What bilinguals tell us about language processing and its consequences for cognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(19), 5577-5583. doi:10.1073/pnas.1419275112

Leikin, M. (2013). The effect of bilingualism on creativity: Developmental and educational perspectives. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(4), 431-447. doi:10.1177/1367006912438300

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., & Howerter, A. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49-100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 187-202. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002

Mulimbi, B. C., & Dryden-Peterson, S. (2013). Bilingualism, language shift, and the educational experiences of students in Africa. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 18(4), 387-400. doi:10.1080/13670050.2013.866629

Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232-258. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(3), 448-460. doi:10.1017/S1355617702813248

Thomas, W. P., & Collier, V. P. (2012). Dual language education for a transformed world. Multilingual Matters. doi:10.21832/9781783090009

Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.

Downloads

Published

2024-07-11

How to Cite

Daud, R. (2024). The Cognitive Benefits of Speaking Multiple Languages. European Journal of Linguistics, 3(4), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.47941/ejl.2055

Issue

Section

Articles