Phenomenology and the Concept of Consciousness

Authors

  • Abigale Cherono Multimedia University

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.47941/ijp.2092

Keywords:

Phenomenology, Consciousness, Intentionality, Embodied Cognition, Neural Correlates

Abstract

Purpose: The general objective of the study was to explore phenomenology and the concept of consciousness.

Methodology: The study adopted a desktop research methodology. Desk research refers to secondary data or that which can be collected without fieldwork. Desk research is basically involved in collecting data from existing resources hence it is often considered a low cost technique as compared to field research, as the main cost is involved in executive’s time, telephone charges and directories. Thus, the study relied on already published studies, reports and statistics. This secondary data was easily accessed through the online journals and library.

Findings: The findings reveal that there exists a contextual and methodological gap relating to phenomenology and the concept of consciousness. Preliminary empirical review revealed a complex relationship between subjective experience and cognitive and neural mechanisms. It demonstrated that phenomenology, focusing on first-person perspectives and intentionality, offers a robust framework for understanding consciousness as an active, embodied engagement with the world. The integration of phenomenological insights with cognitive science and neuroscience provided a comprehensive view, correlating subjective reports with neural activity. This approach proved valuable in clinical settings, enhancing mental health interventions by focusing on individual experiences. The study highlighted the promise of ongoing interdisciplinary research to advance both scientific and philosophical understandings of consciousness.

Unique Contribution to Theory, Practice and Policy: The Theory of Intentionality, Embodies Cognition Theory and Global Workspace Theory may be used to anchor future studies on phenomenology and the concept of consciousness. The study recommended further development of theoretical frameworks integrating phenomenology with cognitive neuroscience, emphasizing the need for interdisciplinary research collaborations. It suggested incorporating phenomenological approaches into mental health practices to enhance patient-centered care and advocated for training healthcare professionals in these methods. The study also advised policymakers to integrate phenomenological findings into mental health policies and prioritize funding for programs that recognize subjective experiences. Lastly, it highlighted the need for future research to explore long-term effects, cultural factors, and new neuroimaging techniques to advance the understanding of consciousness.

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

References

American Psychological Association (APA). (2019). Understanding altered states of consciousness: Mental health statistics. Retrieved from https://www.apa.org/monitor/2019/03/mental-health

Baars, B. J. (2013). A scientific approach to the concept of consciousness. American Journal of Psychology, 126(3), 233-242. https://doi.org/10.5406/amerjpsyc.126.3.0233

Barrett, L. F. (2013). The conceptual act theory: A conceptual framework for the emotions. Emotion Review, 5(4), 348-360. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913484752

Bayne, T., Hohwy, J., & Owen, A. M. (2017). Are There Levels of Consciousness? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(6), 405-413. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.009

Boly, M., Massimini, M., Tsuchiya, N., Postle, B. R., Koch, C., & Tononi, G. (2017). Are the neural correlates of consciousness in the front or in the back of the cerebral cortex? Clinical and neuroimaging evidence. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(40), 9603-9613. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3217-16.2017

Chalmers, D. J. (2013). How can we construct a science of consciousness? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1303(1), 25-35. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12166

Chalmers, D. J. (2013). How can we construct a science of consciousness? Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1303(1), 25-35. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12166

Dreyfus, H. L. (2014). What computers still can’t do: A critique of artificial reason. MIT Press.

Gallagher, S. (2013). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199768268.001.0001

Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2012). The Phenomenological Mind. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203126759

Heidegger, M. (2014). Being and Time. HarperCollins.

Husserl, E. (2012). Ideas: General introduction to pure phenomenology. Routledge.

Koch, C. (2015). Consciousness: Confessions of a romantic reductionist. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262527771.001.0001

Koch, C., Massimini, M., Boly, M., & Tononi, G. (2016). Neural correlates of consciousness: Progress and problems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(5), 307-321. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.22

Lutz, A., Dunne, J. D., & Davidson, R. J. (2013). Meditation and the Neuroscience of Consciousness: An Introduction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1303(1), 5-12. https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.12237

Merleau-Ponty, M. (2014). Phenomenology of Perception. Routledge.

NCCIH. (2017). Use of complementary health approaches in the U.S.: National Health Interview Survey (NHIS). National Center for Complementary and Integrative Health. Retrieved from https://nccih.nih.gov/research/statistics/NHIS

Ndetei, D. M., Khasakhala, L. I., Maru, H. M., Pizzo, M., Mutiso, V., Ongecha-Owuor, F. A., & Kokonya, D. A. (2013). Clinical epidemiology in patients with epilepsy in Kenya. African Journal of Psychiatry, 16(2), 129-134. https://doi.org/10.4314/ajpsy.v16i2.17

Overgaard, M., & Sandberg, K. (2020). Using EEG to Explore the Accuracy of Phenomenological Reports. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 32(4), 601-613. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01511

Palhano-Fontes, F., Andrade, K. C., Tofoli, L. F., Jose, A. C., Crippa, J. A. S., Hallak, J. E. C., ... & de Araujo, D. B. (2015). The psychedelic state induced by ayahuasca modulates the activity and connectivity of the default mode network. PLoS ONE, 10(2), e0118143. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0118143

Petitmengin, C., Remillieux, A., Cahour, B., & Carter-Thomas, S. (2019). A Gap in Nisbett and Wilson’s Findings? A First-Person Access to Our Cognitive Processes. Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 570-585. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2019.02.005

Smith, D. W. (2013). The Cambridge Companion to Husserl. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CCOL9780521675307

Spinelli, E. (2013). The interpreted world: An introduction to phenomenological psychology. Sage Publications.

Stoljar, D. (2013). Phenomenal Intentionality: A Philosophical Introduction. Oxford University Press.

Tagawa, M., Sato, W., & Kakigi, R. (2014). Neural correlates of the meditative state as assessed by magnetoencephalography. PLoS ONE, 9(8), e102962. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102962

Thompson, E., & Varela, F. J. (2012). Radical Embodiment: Neural Dynamics and Consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 111-117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.005

Zahavi, D. (2014). Self-Awareness and Alterity: A Phenomenological Investigation. Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315646394

Downloads

Published

2024-07-13

How to Cite

Cherono, A. (2024). Phenomenology and the Concept of Consciousness. International Journal of Philosophy, 3(3), 59–72. https://doi.org/10.47941/ijp.2092

Issue

Section

Articles